Piaget's moral judgment in a child is read. Jean piagerech and the thinking of a child. Criticism of J. Piaget in Russian psychology

Producer: "ACADEMIC PROJECT"

Series: "Psychological technologies"

480 pages. Based on the study of children's games, the book by J. Piaget explores the formation of moral attitudes in children, the relationship of children's moral judgments with the rules, imperatives that are dictated by adults. The paper draws an important conclusion that is important for modern general sociology and psychology, which helps to determine some of the patterns revealed in the works of modern specialists. ISBN:5-8291-0739-2

Publisher: "ACADEMIC PROJECT" (2006)

Place of Birth:
Date of death:
A place of death:
Citizenship:

Swiss

Scientific area:
Place of work:
Alma mater:

University of Neuchâtel

Known as:

Features of the child's psyche

In the initial period of his activity, Piaget described the features of children's ideas about the world:

  • inseparability of the world and one's own self,
  • animism (belief in the existence of the soul and spirits and in the animation of all nature),
  • artificalism (perception of the world as created by human hands).

To explain them, I used the concept of egocentrism, by which I understood a certain position in relation to the world around, overcome through the process of socialization and affecting the constructions of children's logic: syncretism (connecting everything with everything), non-perception of contradictions, ignoring the general when analyzing the particular, misunderstanding of the relativity of some concepts. All these phenomena find their most vivid expression in egocentric speech.

Theory of intelligence

Later, J. Piaget turned to the study of the intellect, in which he saw the result of the internalization of external actions.

Stages of development of intelligence

Main article: Stages of development of intelligence (J. Piaget)

Piaget identified the following stages in the development of intelligence.

Sensorimotor intelligence (0-2 years)

During the period of sensory-motor intelligence, the organization of perceptual and motor interactions with outside world. This development proceeds from being limited by innate reflexes to the associated organization of sensory-motor actions in relation to the immediate environment. At this stage, only direct manipulations with things are possible, but not actions with symbols, representations in the internal plan.

Preparation and organization of specific operations (2-11 years old)

Sub-period of pre-operational representations (2-7 years)

At the stage of pre-operational representations, a transition is made from sensory-motor functions to internal - symbolic, that is, to actions with representations, and not with external objects.

This stage of the development of the intellect is characterized by the dominance of assumptions and transductive reasoning; egocentrism; centralization on the conspicuous features of the subject and neglect in reasoning of its other features; focusing attention on the states of a thing and inattention to its transformations.

Sub-period of specific operations (7-11 years)

At the stage of specific operations, actions with representations begin to be combined, coordinated with each other, forming systems of integrated actions called operations. The child develops special cognitive structures called factions(For example, classification), thanks to which the child acquires the ability to perform operations with classes and establish logical relationships between classes, uniting them in hierarchies, whereas earlier his abilities were limited to transduction and the establishment of associative links.

The limitation of this stage is that operations can be performed only with concrete objects, but not with statements. Operations logically structure the performed external actions, but they cannot yet structure verbal reasoning in a similar way.

Formal operations (11-15 years old)

The main ability that appears at the stage of formal operations (from 11 to about 15 years old) is the ability to deal with possible, with the hypothetical, and perceive external reality as a special case of what is possible, what could be. Knowledge becomes hypothetical-deductive. The child acquires the ability to think in sentences and establish formal relationships (inclusion, conjunction, disjunction, etc.) between them. The child at this stage is also able to systematically identify all the variables that are essential for solving the problem, and systematically sort through all possible combinations these variables.

Language and thinking

With regard to the relationship between language and thought in cognitive development, Piaget believes that "language does not fully explain thinking, since the structures that characterize this latter are rooted in action and in sensorimotor mechanisms deeper than language reality. Yet it is clear that the more complex the structures of thought become, the more necessary language is to complete their processing. Therefore, language is necessary but not sufficient condition construction of logical operations".

Criticism of J. Piaget in Russian psychology

In the book Thinking and Speech (1934), L. S. Vygotsky entered into a correspondence discussion with Piaget on the question of egocentric speech. Considering Piaget's works as a major contribution to the development of psychological science, L. S. Vygotsky reproached him for the fact that Piaget approached the analysis of the development of higher mental functions in an abstract way, without taking into account the social and cultural environment.

Unfortunately, Piaget was not able to get acquainted with Vygotsky's views until many years after Vygotsky's early death, and it is said that Vygotsky was the only critic of Piaget to whom he responded (from all the rest he only brushed off with the words: "Okay, I agree").

Differences in the views of Piaget and domestic psychologists are manifested in the understanding of the source and driving forces mental development. Piaget viewed mental development as a spontaneous, learning-independent process that obeys biological laws. Domestic psychologists see the source of the child's mental development in his environment, and the development itself is considered as a process of appropriation by the child of socio-historical experience. This explains the role of education in mental development, which is especially emphasized by Russian psychologists and underestimated by Piaget. Critically analyzing the operational concept of intelligence proposed by Piaget, domestic specialists do not consider logic as the only and main criterion of intelligence and do not assess the level of formal operations as highest level development of intellectual activity. Experimental studies(Zaporozhets A. V., Galperin P. Ya., Elkonin D. B.) showed that not logical operations, but orientation in objects and phenomena is the most important part of any human activity and the results of this activity depend on its nature.

Bibliography

  • Piaget J. Selected psychological works. M., 1994.
  • Piaget J. Speech and thinking of the child. M., 1994.
  • Piaget J. Schemes of action and language acquisition // Semiotics. M., 1983. S. 133-136.
  • Piaget J. Genetic aspect of language and thinking // Psycholinguistics. M., 1984.

Books about him

  • Flavell John H. Genetic Psychology of Jean Piaget. M., 1967.
  • Jean Piaget: theory, experiments, discussion / Ed. L. F. Obukhova and G. V. Burmenskaya. Ed. Academica, 2001. ISBN 5-8297-0093-X

T The traditional school prepares more for exams and competitions than for life itself. What has been said, alas, is beyond doubt. Another thing is surprising: these words belong, firstly, not to our contemporary and, secondly, not to a school teacher. They were written more than 70 years ago by J. Piaget, a psychologist with whose name most of us associate research on the development of children's thinking and the discovery of egocentrism. Such ideas are not far from the truth: indeed, Piaget's famous phenomena and their further experimental criticism are most directly related to the problem of the child's mental development.
Having picked up the book Moral Judgment in the Child, the reader has the opportunity to be convinced that Piaget's contribution is not limited to the study of the stages of the cognitive development of the child, but concerns the understanding of the entire course of mental development. Not without reason in the world community Piaget is considered one of the main figures in the genetic psychology of the twentieth century. Unfortunately, it should be noted that this fundamental work by Piaget in 1932 was published without any comments or prefaces. Count on the fact that all who wish to pick up the book are well acquainted with other works by Piaget, as well as with his scientific biography(otherwise, how would they know that the work came out in the first period of his work, which is very important for understanding the author's ideas), at least naively. As a completely predictable consequence, this may have not only a misunderstanding of Piaget's hypothesis about the stages of development of moral ideas in a child, but also an attitude towards his theory as far from life and from the practice of education. And this is contrary to scientific truth.
The book in question deals with the conditions conducive to the intellectual and moral development of the child. J. Piaget showed how cooperation with peers - "work in groups" - becomes a factor in intellectual progress. And the level of cognitive development of the child significantly depends on moral judgments. Piaget studied moral development in the context of children's games by rules, or more precisely, children's understanding of the essence and nature of rules. Preschoolers have no idea that games are played by rules. In junior school age children play by the rules but do not understand that the rules are conventional (with their points of view rules are binding and inviolable). And only at the age of 11-12 years old do children begin to understand that the rules are social conventions and therefore they can be changed if all participants in the game agree with this. Piaget also studied the moral ideas of children, provoking children to reason about the problems of justice and responsibility. These studies were subsequently continued by L. Kohlberg on the material of moral dilemmas.
Piaget's focus was on moral judgments, not moral behavior or feelings, and he saw the danger in getting children to say what we want to hear. But Piaget obtained important results regarding those factors of social life that influence the moral representations of children. And these conclusions, for example, about the role of cooperation as an educational technique, according to Piaget himself, can be used in pedagogy.

From the book Speech and Thinking of a Child the author Piaget Jean

Part I SPEECH AND THINKING OF THE CHILD

From the book Psychology and Psychoanalysis of Character author Raigorodsky Daniil Yakovlevich

CHAPTER IV THE REASONING OF THE CHILD In the preceding chapters we have tried to emphasize various features structures of children's judgment, avoiding, as far as possible, the usual framework of logic textbooks. Indeed, one should not think that one can determine the nature of logic

From the book Fundamentals of the Science of Thinking. Book 1. reasoning author Shevtsov Alexander Alexandrovich

Character and Moral Judgment The problem of moral judgment is often associated with the dichotomy of free will and determinism. According to some, a person is completely determined by various circumstances that he cannot control, so that the idea that a person

From the book After three it's too late by Ibuka Masaru

From the book The Last Child in the Forest author Lowe Richard

From the book Mass and Power author Canetti Elias

From the book Psycholinguistics author Frumkina Revekka Markovna

Part 1. Potential opportunities of the child

From the book Pondered [How to get rid of unnecessary thoughts and focus on the main thing] author Newbigging Sandy

Part IV A Child's Reunion with Nature I'm healthy again, I'm back to life with a cool breeze and a crystal clear mountain stream... John Muir Everyone New Year brings us amazing surprises. We find that we have almost forgotten the sounds of bird songs, and when

From the book Integral Relations author Uchik Martin

From the book Angels Among Us by Virche Dorin

J. Piaget. Fragments from ch. V "Basic Characteristics of the Child's Logic" of the book "Child's Judgment and Reasoning" § 1. Egocentrism in the child's thinking ... Logical activity is a process of proof, a search for truth. When do we feel the need to validate

From the book to Parents: a book of questions and answers. What to do so that children want to learn, know how to make friends and grow up independent author Gippenreiter Yulia Borisovna

Game of Judgment Companions, mind and time, can play a tricky game with your peace and well-being. The mind is able to look into the past and the future, compare and contrast: what was and how it can be done better in the future. This activity I call the game of

From the book FORMATION OF PERSONALITY. LOOK AT PSYCHOTHERAPY by Rogers Carl R.

Judgment and Perception People who are prone to judgment choose an organized and planned approach to life, their existence is orderly. Receptive people prefer a flexible and spontaneous approach to life, as well as openness.

Jean Piaget

Speech and thinking of the child

SPEECH AND THINKING OF THE CHILD

We will try to resolve here next question: what needs does the child seek to satisfy when he speaks? This problem is neither purely linguistic nor purely logical - it is a problem of functional psychology. But it is from this point that any study of the logic of the child must begin.

The question we have posed seems strange at first sight; it seems that in the child, as in ours, speech serves to convey thought. But in fact, it is not so simple at all. First of all, an adult tries to convey various shades of his thoughts with the help of a word. Speech serves him as a statement of thought: words objectively express reflection, provide information and remain associated with knowledge (“the weather is getting worse”, “bodies are falling”, etc.). Sometimes, on the contrary, speech expresses an order or a desire to serve for criticism, threats, in short - for awakening feelings and causing actions (“let's go”, “what a horror!”, etc.). If even approximately one could establish for each individual the relationship between these two categories of transmission, interesting psychological data would be obtained.

But that is not all. Is it possible to assert with certainty that even in an adult, speech always serves to convey, to communicate thoughts? To say nothing of inner speech, a great many - of the people or scattered intellectuals - have a habit of uttering aloud monologues in private. Perhaps this can be seen as a preparation for a public speech: a person who speaks aloud in private sometimes shifts the blame on fictitious interlocutors, like children on the objects of their game. Perhaps there is a "reflected influence of social habits" in this phenomenon, as Baldwin pointed out; the individual repeats in relation to himself a mode of action originally learned by him only in relation to others. In this case, he talks to himself as if in order to force himself to work, he talks because he has already formed the habit of speaking to others in order to influence them. But whether we accept this or that explanation, it is clear that here the function of speech deviates from its purpose: the individual who speaks for himself experiences pleasure and excitement from this, which just distracts him very much from the need to communicate his thoughts to others. Finally, if the function of speech consisted solely in informing, then it would be difficult to explain the phenomenon of verbalism. How can words, intended by their use for exact designations, only existing in order to be understood, could lead to clouding of thought, even to the creation of obscurity, multiplying only verbally existing objects, in short, precisely by making it difficult in many cases to make a thought transmitted? Not wanting to renew discussions here about the relationship between speech and thinking, we only note that the very existence of these discussions proves the complexity of the functions of speech and their irreducibility to a single function - the message of thought.

Thus, the functional problem of speech can be posed even in relation to a normal adult. Moreover, of course, it can be placed in relation to the patient, to a primitive person or to a child. Janet, Freud, Ferenczi, Jones, Spielrein proposed various theories concerning the speech of primitive people, sick and young children - theories that have great importance for the thought of a child of 6 years and older, that is, for the one that we will study.

Janet, for example, believes that the first words come from the cries that accompany an action in animals and primitive man: cries of anger, threats in a fight, etc. For example, the cry with which a commander accompanies a military attack becomes a signal for this attack. Hence the first words - an order. Therefore, the word is first associated with the action of which it is one element, and which is then sufficient to cause this action. Psychoanalysts proceeded from similar ideas to explain the magic of the word. Since the word, by its origin, is a part of the action, it is sufficient to evoke all the mental movements associated with it and all the concrete content.

For example, among the most primitive words, of course, are the cries of love that serve as a preface to sexual intercourse: as a consequence, such words, as well as all words that allude to this act, are endowed with an immediate exciting power. These facts explain the general tendency of primitive thinking to consider the names of things and persons and the designation of events as their very essence. Hence the belief that it is possible to influence these things and events by the mere pronunciation of words; therefore, the word is something much more than ethics; it is the fearsome reality itself, which forms part of the named subject. Spielrein began to look for such phenomena at the very first stages of the child's speech. She tried to prove that the syllables used by the infant to designate the mother in many languages ​​(“mother”) consist of labial consonants, which indicates a simple continuation of the act of sucking.

“Mother”, therefore, is, as it were, at first a cry of desire, and then, in essence, an order that alone can satisfy this desire. But the mere cry of "mother" alone brings some comfort and - since it is a continuation of the act of sucking - some deceptive satisfaction. Command and immediate satisfaction are almost confused here, and it is impossible to know when the word serves as a real command and when it plays its magical role, these two moments are so intertwined here.

Since, for their part, Meiman and Stern showed that the first nouns in a child’s speech do not at all designate concepts, but express orders and desires, then, in the end, there really is reason to believe that the primitive speech of a child is much more complicated than it seems. at first sight. However, even if we treat all the details of these theories with caution, it still becomes obvious that many expressions, which we comprehend simply as concepts, for a small child for a long time have not only an affective meaning, but also a magical one, where everything is connected with special ways of doing things that should be learned for themselves, as they are, not as they appear to adults.

It may therefore be interesting to pose a functional problem with regard to the older child, which is what we would like to do here as an introduction to the study of child logic - logic and speech, which are obviously independent of each other. We may not find any traces of "primitive" phenomena, but at least we will be very far from considering that the child's speech serves to communicate thought, as "common sense" suggests.

There is no need to say how much this experience is under preliminary discussion. We're just trying to probe the ground here. First of all, this should contribute to the creation of a technique suitable for new observations and allowing comparison of results. Such a technique, which we have only so far been looking for, has already allowed us to state something. But since we observed only two children of 6 years old, recording their speech, although completely, but only for one month and during certain hours of the day, we consider our results to be nothing more than preliminary, intending to confirm them in subsequent chapters.

Annotation. The article deals with the problems of the formation of moral judgments and the behavior of a preschool child. The stages in the development of the child's moral judgments, which determine the process of self-regulation of behavior, are characterized. The necessity of using pedagogically expedient evaluative actions that ensure moral development and personal growth preschool child.
Keywords: moral feelings, moral realism, moral judgment, object of evaluation, evaluation activity of a teacher.

At preschool age, an intensive process of mastering the child's ways of behavior and relationships in the surrounding society takes place. The influence of society is multifaceted. Separating right from wrong, good from bad, is a very difficult task for a preschooler. Due to the insufficiently formed experience of life in society, the main way of socialization, moral development is imitation of an adult. The child emotionally reacts to his actions, actions that act as patterns of behavior. Adult ratings are taken unconditionally. It is the experience of communicating with an adult that gives rise to the development in the child of the desire to regulate his relations with the surrounding social world, the mastery of the ability for emotional-evaluative and moral judgments. Moral judgments are expressed in the child's desire to explain their own actions, referring to the norms that adults establish and which oblige them to obey.

Based on the research of J. Piaget, it can be stated that the basis of the entire moral development of the child is the system of his moral judgments. Analyzing the reasoning of children, the researcher concluded that the moral ideas of children change from moral realism to moral relativism.

Moral realism is a firm, unambiguous understanding of good and evil, in which the concept of both justice is confirmed by the authority of an adult. It is the child's tendency to view the duties and values ​​conveyed by adults as binding, regardless of the circumstances. Moral relativism is based on the belief that every person has the right to a fair and respectful attitude towards himself, and in his every act one can see the justified and condemned.

Preschool children, due to the unformed experience of relations with the outside world, cannot question the moral rules that are established by adults, therefore they live and think in terms of moral realism.

First, in the view of the child, an act is good if it does not contradict the rule or requirement of the adult. An act that does not comply with the rules is considered bad. At the same time, the rule itself is formulated clearly, presented as a setting for action, i.e. its execution. The child does not discuss it, because in his view the adult is always right. Therefore, the implementation of these rules is regarded by the child as obedience, a good deed.

Secondly, moral realism is characterized by the observance of the rule, not its content. A literal understanding of the rule by the child leads to the fact that he evaluates the actions of his peers and his own, depending on their compliance with the established rules, without taking into account and not considering as significant those intentions that were their engine. For example, a child wanted to please his beloved teacher and give him a bouquet of flowers. But it turned out that he picked flowers in a flower bed, on the territory kindergarten. Obviously, the method of preparing the bouquet completely overshadowed for peers (perhaps the teacher himself) the child's intention to please the adult.

Evaluation of actions forms the child's moral judgments, which go through several periods in their development.

The first period is characterized by the fact that the concept of fair and unfair is not yet separated from the concept of duty and disobedience: what is right is what meets the requirements of an adult.

However, already in this period, the preschooler is able to highlight the unfair actions of an adult. In particular, this happens when an adult does not follow the rules that he himself has established in relation to the child. For example, in a number of kindergartens, a rule has been established: before going to the play corner, in front of the carpet that covers the floor of the play area, you need to take off your shoes. The rule is set by the teacher. But, it is often possible to observe how educators do not follow this rule. This is immediately noticed by the child as injustice. Therefore, naturally, he has a doubt - why is the rule not for everyone, why does the adult who established it himself not follow the rule? In such situations and similar ones, it is difficult to formulate a convincing answer to the child. The duality of the position of an adult lies in the fact that the requirement to comply with certain norms from children is rejected by him. It is difficult for a preschooler to understand what is a moral norm, and whether it always needs to be followed. But if an adult observes his own rules, the child sees justice in this and seeks to imitate the actions of the educator. This is due to the fact that on this stage authority takes precedence over justice.

The second period in the development of moral judgment is determined only by the age of 7. This period is characterized by the development of independence in judgments, actions, deeds. Fairness in relationships, both with adults and with peers, is a priority for a child. Therefore, a very important category is the interaction of opinions. However, equal interaction between a child and an adult, in its direct sense, is quite difficult, because the inequality in social role positions is obvious. The child tries to imitate the adult and at the same time protect himself from him, rather than exchange opinions. Such relationships are manifested from the moment a new level of interaction between children is established, which has received the definition of "cooperation". It is in this kind of interaction that the child is faced with the need to accept the rules dictated by the children's community. By assimilating and at the same time appropriating them, children can thereby independently regulate their own behavior and actions.

Thanks to the establishment of cooperation relations, there is an awareness of the existence of other points of view on fair and unfair: an adult who is the bearer of the rules he sets himself, and peers who can establish their own rules in joint activities that they themselves organize.

It should be noted that independently organized children's communities are able to regulate relations within it autonomously from an adult. The rules developed jointly are the same for their execution by all participants in a joint game or other activity. In a situation where one of the participants at a certain point in time refuses to comply with the approved rules, he is expelled from the children's community as a violator. And in this action, children see justice. Conversely, if an adult intervenes and evaluates the actions of a group of children as wrong and returns the violator to the game, children are able to defend their rights to justice.

Only with the development of these relations does the mastery of the norms of behavior occur. Such a process is ambiguous and complex for the moral choice of the child. Evaluation of one's own act or the act of a peer is a huge moral work, when it is required not only to correlate the requirements of an adult with a specific situation, but to evaluate it from the point of view of moral rules.

Experience shows that children of senior preschool age are characterized by the dominance of the emotional sphere in the perception of the actions of the people around them. During this period, moral feelings are intensively formed in the child: a sense of camaraderie, responsibility, sympathy for the grief of a comrade, indignation at injustice, etc. Children begin to show an attitude towards themselves as a subject and object of someone else's joy, there is a development of empathic processes that involve entering into the position of another, identification with him. Emotions are social in nature, their content is associated with the moral problems of interpersonal relations. The joy of friendship comes to children, they are afraid to lose friendly relations with others. Experiences are connected with the moral side of interpersonal relations. The desire of children to consider their own actions and the behavior of others through the prism of morality, the criteria for which children borrow from adults, is becoming more and more clearly manifested.

Therefore, it would be wrong to consider two processes separately: the formation of moral judgments of children and their mastery of methods of moral behavior.

The source of obtaining morally enriching knowledge can be a word, a personal example of an educator or an example of people whom the child seeks to imitate. However, on the part of an adult, control over the process of assimilation of moral norms and rules is necessary, since an incorrectly formed idea of ​​\u200b\u200ba particular trait or quality of a person leads to an incorrect line of behavior.

The relative lack of independence of moral thinking and insufficient own experience of children, on the one hand, cause them difficulties in revealing the content of any moral quality and the ability to use moral judgments when characterizing actions, difficulty in consciously operating with moral concepts.

On the other hand, children, at times, cannot do the right things due to the complexity of the life fact, the absence of such cases in their lives or negative influence on them the experience of other people. Moreover, give them the correct moral assessment.

Therefore, the formation of moral behavior based on moral standards can be productive if accompanied by the support and positive assessment of the educator. This not only stimulates the moral activity of the child, but also corrects his behavior, contributes to the correct formation of moral judgments, and enriches his moral experience.

The more accurate the adult's evaluative influence, the more accurate the child's idea of ​​the results of his actions.

Throughout the entire period of preschool childhood, the assessment of an adult, his attitude are of particular importance for the child. In the process of growing up, a preschooler gradually changes his attitude to activities and his behavior, the child is increasingly guided by generally accepted norms and moral rules. However, it is not always possible for a child to adequately evaluate his actions, since an adult for a child is a model of how to do it, and at the same time - a source for evaluating his actions. The child strives to ensure that the adult's assessments coincide with his own. Therefore, it is important for him to understand the essence of evaluative requirements in order to see himself “as if from the outside”, to consciously follow the standards, i.e. manage their behavior, build their relationships with others in different ways.

Self-regulation of behavior as the highest form of normative determination of children's behavior based on moral norms, is formed by the end of preschool childhood and ensures independent and voluntary compliance with moral norms in the absence of external control and coercion.

Pedagogical experience shows that the senior preschool age- a favorable time for the assimilation of many moral norms. In the process of constant accumulation and comprehension of moral facts, relations, their analysis, evaluation, moral decisions are made, responsible choices are made.

  1. Jean Piaget: Moral judgment in the child. Academic project, 2006
  2. Rubtsov VV The role of cooperation in the development of the intellect of children. http://www.voppsy.ru/issues/1980/804/804079.htm